Your browser doesn't support javascript.
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 8 de 8
Filter
1.
Sci Data ; 10(1): 272, 2023 05 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: covidwho-2312761

ABSTRACT

The COVID-19 pandemic has affected all domains of human life, including the economic and social fabric of societies. One of the central strategies for managing public health throughout the pandemic has been through persuasive messaging and collective behaviour change. To help scholars better understand the social and moral psychology behind public health behaviour, we present a dataset comprising of 51,404 individuals from 69 countries. This dataset was collected for the International Collaboration on Social & Moral Psychology of COVID-19 project (ICSMP COVID-19). This social science survey invited participants around the world to complete a series of moral and psychological measures and public health attitudes about COVID-19 during an early phase of the COVID-19 pandemic (between April and June 2020). The survey included seven broad categories of questions: COVID-19 beliefs and compliance behaviours; identity and social attitudes; ideology; health and well-being; moral beliefs and motivation; personality traits; and demographic variables. We report both raw and cleaned data, along with all survey materials, data visualisations, and psychometric evaluations of key variables.


Subject(s)
COVID-19 , Humans , Attitude , COVID-19/psychology , Morals , Pandemics , Surveys and Questionnaires , Social Change , Socioeconomic Factors
2.
Sci Data ; 10(1): 87, 2023 02 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: covidwho-2239406

ABSTRACT

In response to the COVID-19 pandemic, the Psychological Science Accelerator coordinated three large-scale psychological studies to examine the effects of loss-gain framing, cognitive reappraisals, and autonomy framing manipulations on behavioral intentions and affective measures. The data collected (April to October 2020) included specific measures for each experimental study, a general questionnaire examining health prevention behaviors and COVID-19 experience, geographical and cultural context characterization, and demographic information for each participant. Each participant started the study with the same general questions and then was randomized to complete either one longer experiment or two shorter experiments. Data were provided by 73,223 participants with varying completion rates. Participants completed the survey from 111 geopolitical regions in 44 unique languages/dialects. The anonymized dataset described here is provided in both raw and processed formats to facilitate re-use and further analyses. The dataset offers secondary analytic opportunities to explore coping, framing, and self-determination across a diverse, global sample obtained at the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic, which can be merged with other time-sampled or geographic data.


Subject(s)
COVID-19 , Humans , Adaptation, Psychological , Health Behavior , Pandemics , Surveys and Questionnaires
3.
PNAS Nexus ; 1(3): pgac093, 2022 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: covidwho-2234269

ABSTRACT

At the beginning of 2020, COVID-19 became a global problem. Despite all the efforts to emphasize the relevance of preventive measures, not everyone adhered to them. Thus, learning more about the characteristics determining attitudinal and behavioral responses to the pandemic is crucial to improving future interventions. In this study, we applied machine learning on the multinational data collected by the International Collaboration on the Social and Moral Psychology of COVID-19 (N = 51,404) to test the predictive efficacy of constructs from social, moral, cognitive, and personality psychology, as well as socio-demographic factors, in the attitudinal and behavioral responses to the pandemic. The results point to several valuable insights. Internalized moral identity provided the most consistent predictive contribution-individuals perceiving moral traits as central to their self-concept reported higher adherence to preventive measures. Similar results were found for morality as cooperation, symbolized moral identity, self-control, open-mindedness, and collective narcissism, while the inverse relationship was evident for the endorsement of conspiracy theories. However, we also found a non-neglible variability in the explained variance and predictive contributions with respect to macro-level factors such as the pandemic stage or cultural region. Overall, the results underscore the importance of morality-related and contextual factors in understanding adherence to public health recommendations during the pandemic.

4.
Pavlović, Tomislav, Azevedo, Flavio, De, Koustav, Riaño-Moreno, Julián C.; Maglić, Marina, Gkinopoulos, Theofilos, Donnelly-Kehoe, Patricio Andreas, Payán-Gómez, César, Huang, Guanxiong, Kantorowicz, Jaroslaw, Birtel, Michèle D.; Schönegger, Philipp, Capraro, Valerio, Santamaría-García, Hernando, Yucel, Meltem, Ibanez, Agustin, Rathje, Steve, Wetter, Erik, Stanojević, Dragan, van Prooijen, Jan-Willem, Hesse, Eugenia, Elbaek, Christian T.; Franc, Renata, Pavlović, Zoran, Mitkidis, Panagiotis, Cichocka, Aleksandra, Gelfand, Michele, Alfano, Mark, Ross, Robert M.; Sjåstad, Hallgeir, Nezlek, John B.; Cislak, Aleksandra, Lockwood, Patricia, Abts, Koen, Agadullina, Elena, Amodio, David M.; Apps, Matthew A. J.; Aruta, John Jamir Benzon, Besharati, Sahba, Bor, Alexander, Choma, Becky, Cunningham, William, Ejaz, Waqas, Farmer, Harry, Findor, Andrej, Gjoneska, Biljana, Gualda, Estrella, Huynh, Toan L. D.; Imran, Mostak Ahamed, Israelashvili, Jacob, Kantorowicz-Reznichenko, Elena, Krouwel, André, Kutiyski, Yordan, Laakasuo, Michael, Lamm, Claus, Levy, Jonathan, Leygue, Caroline, Lin, Ming-Jen, Mansoor, Mohammad Sabbir, Marie, Antoine, Mayiwar, Lewend, Mazepus, Honorata, McHugh, Cillian, Olsson, Andreas, Otterbring, Tobias, Packer, Dominic, Palomäki, Jussi, Perry, Anat, Petersen, Michael Bang, Puthillam, Arathy, Rothmund, Tobias, Schmid, Petra C.; Stadelmann, David, Stoica, Augustin, Stoyanov, Drozdstoy, Stoyanova, Kristina, Tewari, Shruti, Todosijević, Bojan, Torgler, Benno, Tsakiris, Manos, Tung, Hans H.; Umbreș, Radu Gabriel, Vanags, Edmunds, Vlasceanu, Madalina, Vonasch, Andrew J.; Zhang, Yucheng, Abad, Mohcine, Adler, Eli, Mdarhri, Hamza Alaoui, Antazo, Benedict, Ay, F. Ceren, Ba, Mouhamadou El Hady, Barbosa, Sergio, Bastian, Brock, Berg, Anton, Białek, Michał, Bilancini, Ennio, Bogatyreva, Natalia, Boncinelli, Leonardo, Booth, Jonathan E.; Borau, Sylvie, Buchel, Ondrej, de Carvalho, Chrissie Ferreira, Celadin, Tatiana, Cerami, Chiara, Chalise, Hom Nath, Cheng, Xiaojun, Cian, Luca, Cockcroft, Kate, Conway, Jane, Córdoba-Delgado, Mateo A.; Crespi, Chiara, Crouzevialle, Marie, Cutler, Jo, Cypryańska, Marzena, Dabrowska, Justyna, Davis, Victoria H.; Minda, John Paul, Dayley, Pamala N.; Delouvée, Sylvain, Denkovski, Ognjan, Dezecache, Guillaume, Dhaliwal, Nathan A.; Diato, Alelie, Di Paolo, Roberto, Dulleck, Uwe, Ekmanis, Jānis, Etienne, Tom W.; Farhana, Hapsa Hossain, Farkhari, Fahima, Fidanovski, Kristijan, Flew, Terry, Fraser, Shona, Frempong, Raymond Boadi, Fugelsang, Jonathan, Gale, Jessica, García-Navarro, E. Begoña, Garladinne, Prasad, Gray, Kurt, Griffin, Siobhán M.; Gronfeldt, Bjarki, Gruber, June, Halperin, Eran, Herzon, Volo, Hruška, Matej, Hudecek, Matthias F. C.; Isler, Ozan, Jangard, Simon, Jørgensen, Frederik, Keudel, Oleksandra, Koppel, Lina, Koverola, Mika, Kunnari, Anton, Leota, Josh, Lermer, Eva, Li, Chunyun, Longoni, Chiara, McCashin, Darragh, Mikloušić, Igor, Molina-Paredes, Juliana, Monroy-Fonseca, César, Morales-Marente, Elena, Moreau, David, Muda, Rafał, Myer, Annalisa, Nash, Kyle, Nitschke, Jonas P.; Nurse, Matthew S.; de Mello, Victoria Oldemburgo, Palacios-Galvez, Maria Soledad, Pan, Yafeng, Papp, Zsófia, Pärnamets, Philip, Paruzel-Czachura, Mariola, Perander, Silva, Pitman, Michael, Raza, Ali, Rêgo, Gabriel Gaudencio, Robertson, Claire, Rodríguez-Pascual, Iván, Saikkonen, Teemu, Salvador-Ginez, Octavio, Sampaio, Waldir M.; Santi, Gaia Chiara, Schultner, David, Schutte, Enid, Scott, Andy, Skali, Ahmed, Stefaniak, Anna, Sternisko, Anni, Strickland, Brent, Thomas, Jeffrey P.; Tinghög, Gustav, Traast, Iris J.; Tucciarelli, Raffaele, Tyrala, Michael, Ungson, Nick D.; Uysal, Mete Sefa, Van Rooy, Dirk, Västfjäll, Daniel, Vieira, Joana B.; von Sikorski, Christian, Walker, Alexander C.; Watermeyer, Jennifer, Willardt, Robin, Wohl, Michael J. A.; Wójcik, Adrian Dominik, Wu, Kaidi, Yamada, Yuki, Yilmaz, Onurcan, Yogeeswaran, Kumar, Ziemer, Carolin-Theresa, Zwaan, Rolf A.; Boggio, Paulo Sergio, Whillans, Ashley, Van Lange, Paul A. M.; Prasad, Rajib, Onderco, Michal, O'Madagain, Cathal, Nesh-Nash, Tarik, Laguna, Oscar Moreda, Kubin, Emily, Gümren, Mert, Fenwick, Ali, Ertan, Arhan S.; Bernstein, Michael J.; Amara, Hanane, Van Bavel, Jay Joseph.
PNAS nexus ; 1(3), 2022.
Article in English | EuropePMC | ID: covidwho-1989908

ABSTRACT

At the beginning of 2020, COVID-19 became a global problem. Despite all the efforts to emphasize the relevance of preventive measures, not everyone adhered to them. Thus, learning more about the characteristics determining attitudinal and behavioral responses to the pandemic is crucial to improving future interventions. In this study, we applied machine learning on the multinational data collected by the International Collaboration on the Social and Moral Psychology of COVID-19 (N = 51,404) to test the predictive efficacy of constructs from social, moral, cognitive, and personality psychology, as well as socio-demographic factors, in the attitudinal and behavioral responses to the pandemic. The results point to several valuable insights. Internalized moral identity provided the most consistent predictive contribution—individuals perceiving moral traits as central to their self-concept reported higher adherence to preventive measures. Similar results were found for morality as cooperation, symbolized moral identity, self-control, open-mindedness, and collective narcissism, while the inverse relationship was evident for the endorsement of conspiracy theories. However, we also found a non-neglible variability in the explained variance and predictive contributions with respect to macro-level factors such as the pandemic stage or cultural region. Overall, the results underscore the importance of morality-related and contextual factors in understanding adherence to public health recommendations during the pandemic.

5.
Curr Opin Behav Sci ; 48: 101208, 2022 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: covidwho-1966448

ABSTRACT

People from low- and middle-income countries (LMICs) represent large portions of the world population, often occupy less favorable living conditions, and typically suffer greater health risks, yet frequently receive little research and global health attention. The present study reviews emerging evidence on problematic use of the Internet (PUI) in LMICs prior/during the COVID-19 pandemic. Analyzed studies mainly focused on general properties of PUI in university students, problematic gaming in youth, or problematic use of social media in adults, registering higher prevalence estimates, as compared with earlier reports. Research mainly focused on initially affected regions and COVID-exposed populations. Overall, unfavorable circumstances, including poor social support, family relationships, and lifestyle tendencies/habits, may present potential risk for PUI in LMICs, likely exacerbated during the pandemic.

6.
Nat Commun ; 13(1): 517, 2022 01 26.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: covidwho-1655570

ABSTRACT

Changing collective behaviour and supporting non-pharmaceutical interventions is an important component in mitigating virus transmission during a pandemic. In a large international collaboration (Study 1, N = 49,968 across 67 countries), we investigated self-reported factors associated with public health behaviours (e.g., spatial distancing and stricter hygiene) and endorsed public policy interventions (e.g., closing bars and restaurants) during the early stage of the COVID-19 pandemic (April-May 2020). Respondents who reported identifying more strongly with their nation consistently reported greater engagement in public health behaviours and support for public health policies. Results were similar for representative and non-representative national samples. Study 2 (N = 42 countries) conceptually replicated the central finding using aggregate indices of national identity (obtained using the World Values Survey) and a measure of actual behaviour change during the pandemic (obtained from Google mobility reports). Higher levels of national identification prior to the pandemic predicted lower mobility during the early stage of the pandemic (r = -0.40). We discuss the potential implications of links between national identity, leadership, and public health for managing COVID-19 and future pandemics.


Subject(s)
Pandemics/legislation & jurisprudence , Public Health/legislation & jurisprudence , Social Conformity , COVID-19/epidemiology , COVID-19/prevention & control , COVID-19/psychology , Cross-Cultural Comparison , Health Behavior , Humans , Leadership , Pandemics/prevention & control , Pandemics/statistics & numerical data , SARS-CoV-2 , Self Report , Social Identification
7.
Front Psychol ; 12: 736838, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: covidwho-1497144

ABSTRACT

The tendency to believe in conspiracy theories (implying secret and malevolent plots by scheming groups or individuals), incites growing decennial interest among psychological researchers (exploring the associated personality traits, worldviews and cognitive styles of people). The link between the conspiratorial beliefs and the cognitive styles remains of particular interest to scholars, requiring integrated theoretical considerations. This perspective article will focus on the relationship between the propensity to (dis)trust conspiracy theories and three cognitive styles: analytic thinking, critical thinking, and scientific reasoning. Analytic thinking (inclination toward slow and deliberate processing of information in a conscious effort to mitigate biases and reach objective understanding of facts), is a well-studied concept in the context of conspiratorial beliefs, while the negative mutual relationship seems well-evidenced. On the other hand, the evidence on the link with the critical thinking (readiness to consider, reason, appraise, review, and interpret facts to update existing beliefs) has only started to emerge in the last years. Finally, scientific reasoning (ability to apply principles of scientific inquiry to formulate, test, revise and update knowledge in accordance with new evidence), is the least studied of the three cognitive styles in relation to conspiracy theories. The present article will: (a) revise the (lack of) scientific consensus on the definitional and conceptual aspects (by providing theoretical framework); (b) summarize the state of the art on the subject (by providing overview of empirical evidence); (c) discuss directions for future research (especially in relation to the COVID-19 pandemic). An integrated perspective on the relationship between conspiratorial beliefs and cognitive styles of people, may serve to inspire future behavioral interventions.

8.
Compr Psychiatry ; 112: 152279, 2022 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: covidwho-1471932

ABSTRACT

With the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic and the accelerated spread of the SARS-CoV-2 virus came jurisdictional limitations on mobility of citizens and distinct alterations in their daily routines. Confined to their homes, many people increased their overall internet use, with problematic use of the internet (PUI) becoming a potential reason for increased mental health concerns. Our narrative review summarizes information on the extent of PUI during the pandemic, by focusing on three types: online gaming, gambling and pornography viewing. We conclude by providing guidance for mental health professionals and those affected by PUI (with an outline of immediate research priorities and best therapeutic approaches), as well as for the general public (with an overview of safe and preventative practices).


Subject(s)
COVID-19 , Humans , Internet , Mental Health , Pandemics/prevention & control , SARS-CoV-2
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL